JIJITANG2014-07-02 4:09 PM

TASTY METAPHORS SPARK BRAIN’S ‘EMOTIONAL CENTERS’

We’re so used to metaphors about tastes that when we hear a kind smile called “sweet” or a nasty comment called “bitter,” the descriptions almost seem literal. New research shows that taste-related metaphors actually engage the emotional centers of the brain more than literal words with the same meaning.

A new study in the Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience is the first to experimentally show that the brain processes these everyday metaphors differently than literal language.

In the study, participants read 37 sentences that included common metaphors based on taste while the researchers recorded their brain activity. Each taste-related word was then swapped with a literal counterpart so that, for instance, “She looked at him sweetly” became “She looked at him kindly.”

The researchers found that the sentences containing words that invoked taste-activated areas known to be associated with emotional processing, such as the amygdala, as well as the areas known as the gustatory cortices that allow for the physical act of tasting.

Interestingly, the metaphorical and literal words only resulted in brain activity related to emotion when part of a sentence, but stimulated the gustatory cortices both in sentences and as stand-alone words.

‘SWEET’ VS. ‘KIND’
Metaphorical sentences may spark increased brain activity in emotion-related regions because they allude to physical experiences, says coauthor Adele Goldberg, a professor of linguistics at Princeton University.

Human language frequently uses physical sensations or objects to refer to abstract domains such as time, understanding, or emotion, Goldberg says. For instance, people liken love to a number of afflictions including being “sick” or shot through the heart with an arrow.

Similarly, “sweet” has a much clearer physical component than “kind.” The new research suggests that these associations go beyond just being descriptive to engage our brains on an emotional level and potentially amplify the impact of the sentence, Goldberg says.

“You begin to realize when you look at metaphors how common they are in helping us understand abstract domains,” Goldberg says. “It could be that we are more engaged with abstract concepts when we use metaphorical language that ties into physical experiences.”

EMOTIONAL IMPACT

If metaphors in general elicit an emotional response from the brain that is similar to that caused by taste-related metaphors, then that could mean that figurative language presents a “rhetorical advantage” when communicating with others, explains coauthor Francesca Citron, a postdoctoral researcher of psycholinguistics at the Free University of Berlin’s Languages of Emotion research center.

“Figurative language may be more effective in communication and may facilitate processes such as affiliation, persuasion, and support,” Citron says. “Further, as a reader or listener, one should be wary of being overly influenced by metaphorical language.”


Colloquially, metaphors seem to be employed precisely to evoke an emotional reaction, yet the actual emotional effect of figurative phrases on the person hearing them has not before been deeply explored, says Benjamin Bergen, an associate professor of cognitive science at the University of California, San Diego, who studies language comprehension, and metaphorical language and thought.


“There’s a lot of research on the conceptual effects of metaphors, such as how they allow people to think about new or abstract concepts in terms of concrete things they’re familiar with. But there’s very little work on the emotional impact of metaphor,” says Bergen, who had no role in the research but is familiar with it.


“Emotional impact seems to be one of the main reasons people use metaphors to begin with. For instance, a senator might describe a bill as ‘job-killing’ to evoke an emotional reaction,” he says. “These results suggest that using certain metaphorical expressions induces more of an emotional reaction than saying the same thing literally. Those expressions that have this property are likely to have the effects on reasoning, inference, judgment, and decision-making that emotion is known to have.”


BRAIN ACTIVITY


The brain areas that taste-related words did not stimulate are also an important outcome of the study, Citron says. Existing research on metaphors and neural processing has shown that figurative language generally requires more brainpower than literal language, Citron and Goldberg write. But these bursts of neural activity have been related to higher-order processing from thinking through an unfamiliar metaphor.


The brain activity Citron and Goldberg observed did not correlate with this process. In order to create the metaphorical- and literal-sentence stimuli, they had a group of people separate from the study participants rate sentences for familiarity, apparent arousal, imageability—which is how easily a phrase can be imagined in the reader’s mind—and how positive or negative each sentence was interpreted as being.


The metaphorical and literal sentences were equal on all of these factors. In addition, each metaphorical phrase and its literal counterpart were rated as being highly similar in meaning.


These steps helped to ensure that the metaphorical and literal sentences were equally as easy to comprehend. Thus, the brain activity the researchers recorded was not likely to be in response to any additional difficulty study participants had in understanding the metaphors.


“It is important to rule out possible effects of familiarity, since less familiar items may require more processing resources to be understood and elicit enhanced brain responses in several brain regions,” Citron says.


Citron and Goldberg plan to follow up on their results by examining if figurative language is remembered more accurately than literal language, if metaphors are more physically stimulating, and if metaphors related to other senses also provoke an emotional response from the brain.


An Einstein Visiting Fellowship from the Einstein Foundation in Berlin supported the work.


Original Article
<Metaphorical Sentences Are More Emotionally Engaging than Their Literal Counterparts>, Published on Journal <MIT Press Journals> in June 2014

KEYWORDS

SHARE & LIKE

COMMENTS

ABOUT THE AUTHOR

JIJITANG

0 Following 3 Fans 0 Projects 310 Articles

SIMILAR ARTICLES

Changes to the Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children (WIC) have resulted in families eating more fruits and vegetable

Read More

In contests drawn from game theory, chimpanzee pairs consistently outperform humans in games that test memory and strategic thinking.A new study, condu

Read More

Publishing is one of the most ballyhooed metrics of scientific careers, and every researcher hates to have a gap in that part of his or her CV. Here’s

Read More

Pornography triggers brain activity in people with compulsive sexual behaviour -- known commonly as sex addiction -- similar to that triggered by drugs

Read More

In a recent survey, academic staff at the University identified the interrelated skills of writing and reasoning as the two most important skills for s

Read More

In a recent survey, academic staff at the University identified the interrelated skills of writing and reasoning as the two most important skills for s

Read More

Reading cautionary tales like The Boy Who Cried Wolf and Pinocchio to little kids might not be the best way to teach them to tell the truth.New researc

Read More

Those flat, glassy solar panels on your neighbor’s roof may be getting a more efficient makeover, thanks to a new class of solar-sensitive nanoparticle

Read More

Physicists have overcome a major challenge in the science of measurement using quantum mechanics. They’ve used multiple detectors to measure photons in

Read More

If you want to slow down long enough to smell the proverbial roses, you might want to move to a neighborhood with fewer drive-thru restaurants, researc

Read More